top of page

Why We Dream: The Psychological Theories Behind Sleep and the Subconscious

  • Manos Podaras
  • Apr 3
  • 3 min read

ree


Dreaming has fascinated psychologists, neuroscientists, and philosophers for centuries.

Despite advances in sleep research, the purpose and function of dreams remain a subject

of debate. Various psychological theories attempt to explain why we dream, linking

dreams to memory consolidation, emotional processing, problem-solving, and

unconscious desires. This article explores the major psychological theories behind

dreaming and its connection to the subconscious mind.


The Psychoanalytic Perspective


Sigmund Freud’s (1900) psychoanalytic theory posits that dreams serve as a gateway

to the unconscious, revealing repressed desires, conflicts, and unresolved emotions.

According to Freud, dreams operate through mechanisms such as condensation,

displacement, and symbolic representation, allowing individuals to express

unconscious wishes in a disguised form. The manifest content (what we consciously

recall) differs from the latent content (hidden psychological meaning), requiring

interpretation. While Freud’s theory has been criticized for its lack of empirical support,

it remains influential in psychoanalysis and dream interpretation (Schredl, 2003).


The Activation-Synthesis Model


Hobson and McCarley (1977) proposed the activation-synthesis theory, which suggests

that dreams result from random neural activity during REM sleep. According to this

model, the brainstem generates spontaneous electrical signals, which the cerebral cortex

attempts to synthesize into a coherent narrative. This theory challenges the notion that

dreams have intrinsic meaning, arguing instead that they are byproducts of

neurobiological processes. While this model is widely accepted in neuroscience, it does

not account for the emotional and thematic coherence often found in dreams (Domhoff,

2005).


The Cognitive Perspective


Cognitive theories propose that dreaming plays a crucial role in memory consolidation

and problem-solving. Research by Stickgold and Walker (2013) suggests that REM

sleep facilitates the integration of newly acquired information, enhancing learning and

cognitive flexibility. Dreams may also serve as simulations, allowing individuals to

rehearse real-life scenarios and process emotional experiences. The continuity

hypothesis, supported by Hartmann (2014), argues that dreams reflect waking-life

concerns and emotions, bridging the gap between consciousness and the subconscious

mind.


The Threat Simulation Theory


Revonsuo (2000) introduced the threat simulation theory, which suggests that dreaming

evolved as an adaptive function to rehearse survival strategies. This theory argues that

the brain simulates threatening situations in dreams to prepare individuals for real-life

dangers. Empirical studies have shown that people who experience trauma often haverecurrent nightmares, supporting the idea that dreaming helps process fear-related

memories and enhances emotional regulation (Levin & Nielsen, 2009).


The Role of Dreams in Emotional Processing


Dreams may contribute to emotional regulation by helping individuals process

unresolved emotions and reduce stress. Research indicates that REM sleep is associated

with decreased activity in the amygdala, a brain region involved in emotional responses

(Walker & van der Helm, 2009). This suggests that dreaming aids in emotional

adaptation, allowing individuals to reframe distressing experiences in a less reactive

manner.


Conclusion


Although no single theory fully explains why we dream, psychological perspectives

suggest that dreams serve multiple functions, including emotional processing, memory

consolidation, and unconscious expression. Understanding the role of dreaming may

offer valuable insights into mental health, cognitive function, and the subconscious

mind. As research in sleep psychology advances, future studies may further illuminate

the intricate relationship between dreaming and human cognition.


References

Domhoff, G. W. (2005). Refocusing the neurocognitive approach to dreams:
The continuity hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6), 951-952.

Hartmann, E. (2014). The nature and functions of dreaming. Oxford: Oxford
University Press

Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream state
generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 134(12), 1335-1348.

Levin, R., & Nielsen, T. A. (2009). Nightmares, bad dreams, and emotion
dysregulation: A review and new neurocognitive model. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 32(6), 561-579.

Revonsuo, A. (2000). The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary
hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
23(6), 877-901.

Schredl, M., (2003). Continuity between waking and dreaming: A proposal
for a mathematical model. Sleep and Hypnosis, 5(1), 38-52.

Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2013). Sleep-dependent memory triage:
Evolving generalization through selective processing. Nature Neuroscience,
16(2), 139-145.

Walker, M. P., & van der Helm, E. (2009). Overnight therapy? The role of
sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 731-748.
 
 
 

Comments


Commenting on this post isn't available anymore. Contact the site owner for more info.
bottom of page